The 48 Laws of Power (2000) is the first book
by American author Robert Greene. The book, an international bestseller, is a
practical guide for anyone who wants power, observes power, or wants to arm
himself against power. It has sold over 1.2 million copies in the United States
alone and is popular with famous rappers, entrepreneurs, celebrities, athletes
and actors including 50 Cent, Jay-Z, Kanye West, Busta Rhymes, Ludacris, DJ
Premier, Dov Charney, Brian Grazer, Andrew Bynum, Chris Bosh, and Will Smith. It
has also found its way into the warm embrace of the power wielders within the Nigerian
power corridors and among various helms men and women piloting political
affairs, university administration and church leadership. The categorization of the laws as well as its enumeration
is as follows:
|
|
His view and pontificating
thrust in the Power demystification research effort is here harvested for
your reading delight and further objective criticism:
“It is much safer to be
feared than loved,” writes Niccolò Machiavelli in The Prince – his classic
16th-century treatise advocating manipulation and occasional cruelty as the
best means to power.
Guided by centuries of
advice like Machiavelli’s and Greene’s, we tend to believe that attaining power
requires force, deception, manipulation, and coercion.
Indeed, we might even assume that positions of power demand this kind of
conduct—that, to run smoothly, society needs leaders who are willing and able
to use power this way.
As seductive as these
notions are, they are dead wrong. Instead, a new science of power has revealed
that power is wielded most effectively when it’s used responsibly by people
who are attuned to, and engaged with the needs and interests of others.
The power paradox
requires that we be ever vigilant against the corruptive influences of power
and its ability to distort the way we see ourselves and treat others. But this
paradox also makes clear how important it is to challenge myths about power,
which persuade us to choose the wrong kinds of leaders and to tolerate gross
abuses of power. Instead of succumbing to the Machiavellian worldview—which unfortunately
leads us to select Machiavellian leaders—we must promote a different model of
power, one rooted in social intelligence, responsibility, and cooperation.
Myth number one: Power equals cash, votes,
and muscle.
The term “power” often
evokes images of force and coercion. Many people assume that power is most
evident on the floor of the United States Congress or in corporate boardrooms.
Treatments of power in the social sciences have followed suit, zeroing in on
clashes over cash (financial wealth), votes (participation in the political
decision making process), and muscle (military might).
But there are innumerable
exceptions to this definition of power: a penniless two year old pleading for
(and getting) candy in the check-out line at the grocery store, one spouse
manipulating another for sex, or the success of nonviolent political movements
in places like India or South Africa. Viewing power as cash, votes, and muscle
blinds us to the ways power pervades our daily lives.
New psychological
research has redefined power, and this definition makes clear just how prevalent
and integral power is in all of our lives. In psychological science, power is
defined as one’s capacity to alter another person’s condition or state of mind
by providing or withholding resources— such as food, money, knowledge, and
affection—or administering punishments, such as physical harm, job termination,
or social ostracism.
This definition
de-emphasizes how a person actually acts, and instead stresses the individual’s
capacity to affect others. Perhaps most importantly, this definition applies
across relationships, contexts, and cultures. It helps us understand how
children can wield power over their parents from the time they’re born, or how
someone—say, a religious leader—can be powerful in one context (on the pulpit
during a Sunday sermon) but not another (on a mind numbingly slow line at the
DMV come Monday morning). By this definition, one can be powerful without
needing to try to control, coerce, or dominate. Indeed, when people resort
to trying to control others, it’s often a sign that their power is slipping.
This definition
complicates our understanding of power. Power is not something limited to
power-hungry individuals or organizations; it is part of every social
interaction where people have the capacity to influence one another’s states,
which is really every moment of life. Claims that power is simply a product of
male biology miss the degree to which women have obtained and wielded power in
many social situations. In fact, studies I’ve conducted find that people grant
power to women as readily as men, and in informal social hierarchies, women
achieve similar levels of power as men.
So power is not something
we should (or can) avoid, nor is it something that necessarily involves
domination and submission. We are negotiating power every waking instant of our
social lives (and in our dreams as well, Freud argued). When we seek equality,
we are seeking an effective balance of power, not the absence of power. We use
it to win consent and social cohesion, not just compliance. To be human is to
be immersed in power dynamics.
Myth number two: Machiavellians win in the
game of power.
One of the central
questions concerning power is who gets it. Researchers have confronted this
question for years, and their results offer a sharp rebuke to the Machiavellian
view of power. It is not the manipulative, strategic Machiavellian who rises in
power. Instead, social science reveals that one’s ability to get or maintain
power, even in small group situations, depends on one’s ability to understand
and advance the goals of other group members. When it comes to power, social
intelligence—reconciling conflicts, negotiating, smoothing over group tensions—prevails
over social Darwinism. For instance, highly detailed studies of “chimpanzee
politics” have found that social power among non-human primates is based less
on sheer strength, coercion, and the unbridled assertion of self-interest, and
more on the ability to negotiate conflicts, to enforce group norms, and to
allocate resources fairly. More often than not, this research shows, primates
who try to wield their power by dominating others and prioritizing their own
interests will find themselves challenged and, in time, deposed by
subordinates. (Christopher Boehm describes this research in greater length in his
essay.)
In my own research on
human social hierarchies, I have consistently found that it is the more
dynamic, playful, engaging members of the group who quickly garner and maintain
the respect of their peers. Such outgoing, energetic, socially engaged individuals
quickly rise through the ranks of emerging hierarchies.
Why social intelligence?
Because of our ultra-sociability. We accomplish most tasks related to survival
and reproduction socially, from caring for our children to producing food and
shelter. We give power to those who can best serve the interests of the group.
Time and time again,
empirical studies find that leaders who treat their subordinates with respect,
share power, and generate a sense of camaraderie and trust are considered more
just and fair. Social intelligence is essential not only to rising to power,
but to keeping it.
My colleague Cameron
Anderson and I have studied the structure of social hierarchies within college
dormitories over the course of a year, examining who is at the top and remains
there, who falls in status, and who is less well-respected by their peers.
We’ve consistently found that it is the socially engaged individuals who keep
their power over time. In more recent work, Cameron has made the
remarkable discovery that modesty may be critical to maintaining power.
Individuals who are modest about their own power actually rise in hierarchies
and maintain the status and respect of their peers, while individuals with an
inflated, grandiose sense of power quickly fall to the bottom rungs.
So what is the fate of
Machiavellian group members, avid practitioners of Greene’s 48 laws, who are
willing to deceive, backstab, intimidate, and undermine others in their pursuit
of power? We’ve found that these individuals do not actually rise to positions
of power. Instead, their peers quickly recognize that they will harm others in
the pursuit of their own self-interest, and tag them with a reputation of being
harmful to the group and not worthy of leadership.
Cooperation and modesty
aren’t just ethical ways to use power, and they don’t only serve the interests
of a group; they’re also valuable skills for people who seek positions of power
and want to hold onto them.
Myth number
three: Power
is strategically acquired, not given.
A major reason why
Machiavellians fail is that they fall victim to a third myth about power. They
mistakenly believe that power is acquired strategically in deceptive
gamesmanship and by pitting others against one another. Here Machiavelli failed
to appreciate an important fact in the evolution of human hierarchies: that
with increasing social intelligence, subordinates can form powerful alliances
and constrain the actions of those in power. Power increasingly has come to
rest on the actions and judgments of other group members. A person’s power is
only as strong as the status given to that person by others.The sociologist Erving Goffman wrote with brilliant insight about deference— the manner in which we afford power to others with honorifics, formal prose, indirectness, and modest nonverbal displays of embarrassment. We can give power to others simply by being respectfully polite.
My own research has found
that people instinctively identify individuals who might undermine the
interests of the group, and prevent those people from rising in power, through
what we call “reputational discourse.” In our research on different groups, we
have asked group members to talk openly about other members’ reputations and to
engage in gossip. We’ve found that Machiavellians quickly acquire reputations
as individuals who act in ways that are inimical to the interests of others, and
these reputations act like a glass ceiling, preventing their rise in power. In
fact, this aspect of their behavior affected their reputations even more than
their sexual morality, recreational habits, or their willingness to abide by
group social conventions.
The power paradox
“Power tends to corrupt;
absolute power corrupts absolutely,” said the British historian Lord Acton.
Unfortunately, this is not entirely a myth, as the actions of Europe’s
monarchs, Enron’s executives, and out-of- control pop stars reveal. A great
deal of research—especially from social psychology— lends support to Acton’s
claim, albeit with a twist: Power leads people to act in impulsive fashion,
both good and bad, and to fail to understand other people’s feelings and
desires. For instance, studies have found that people given power in
experiments are more likely to rely on stereotypes when judging others, and
they pay less attention to the characteristics that define those other people
as individuals. Predisposed to stereotype, they also judge others’ attitudes,
interests, and needs less accurately. One survey found that high-power
professors made less accurate judgments about the attitudes of low-power
professors than those low-power professors made about the attitudes of their
more powerful colleagues. Power imbalances may even help explain the finding
that older siblings don’t perform as well as their younger siblings on
theory-of-mind tasks, which assess one’s ability to construe the intentions and
beliefs of others. Power even prompts less
complex legal reasoning in Supreme Court justices.
A study led by Stanford
psychologist Deborah Gruenfeld compared the decisions of U.S. Supreme Court justices
when they wrote opinions endorsing either the position of a majority of
justices on the bench—a position of power—or the position of the vanquished,
less powerful minority.
Sure enough, when
Gruenfeld analyzed the complexity of justices’ opinions on a vast array of
cases, she found that justices writing from a position of power crafted less
complex arguments than those writing from a low-power position.
A great deal of research has also found that power encourages individuals to act on their own whims, desires, and impulses. When researchers give people power in scientific experiments, those people are more likely to physically touch others in potentially inappropriate ways, to flirt in more direct fashion, to make risky choices and gambles, to make first offers in negotiations, to speak their mind, and to eat cookies like the Cookie Monster, with crumbs all over their chins and chests.
Perhaps more unsettling
is the wealth of evidence that having power makes people more likely to act
like sociopaths. High-power individuals are more likely to interrupt others, to
speak out of turn, and to fail to look at others who are speaking. They are
also more likely to tease friends and colleagues in hostile, humiliating
fashion. Surveys of organizations find that most rude behaviors—shouting,
profanities, bald critiques—emanate from the offices and cubicles of
individuals in positions of power.
My own research has found
that people with power tend to behave like patients who have damaged their
brain’s orbitofrontal lobes (the region of the frontal lobes right behind the
eye sockets), a condition that seems to cause overly impulsive and insensitive
behavior. Thus the experience of power might be thought of as having someone
open up your skull and take out that part of your brain so critical to empathy
and socially-appropriate behavior.
Power may induce more
harmful forms of aggression as well. In the famed Stanford Prison Experiment,
psychologist Philip Zimbardo randomly assigned Stanford undergraduates to act
as prison guards or prisoners—an extreme kind of power relation. The prison guards
quickly descended into the purest forms of power abuse, psychologically torturing
their peers, the prisoners. Similarly, anthropologists have found that cultures
where rape is prevalent and accepted tend to be cultures with deeply entrenched
beliefs in the supremacy of men over women. This leaves us with a power
paradox. Power is given to those individuals, groups, or nations who advance
the interests of the greater good in socially intelligent fashion.
Yet unfortunately, having
power renders many individuals as impulsive and poorly attuned to others as
your garden-variety frontal lobe patient, making them prone to act abusively
and lose the esteem of their peers. What people want from leaders—social
intelligence—is what is damaged by the experience of power.
When we recognize this
paradox and all the destructive behaviors that flow from it, we can appreciate
the importance of promoting a more socially intelligent model of power. Social
behaviors are dictated by social expectations. As we debunk long-standing myths
and misconceptions about power, we can better identify the qualities powerful
people should have, and better understand how they should wield their power. As
a result, we’ll have much less tolerance for people who lead by deception,
coercion, or undue force. No longer will we expect these kinds of antisocial
behaviors from our leaders and silently accept them when they come to pass.
We’ll also start to
demand something more from our colleagues, our neighbors, and ourselves. When
we appreciate the distinctions between responsible and irresponsible uses of
power—and the importance of practicing the responsible, socially-intelligent form
of it—we take a vital step toward promoting healthy marriages, peaceful
playgrounds, and societies built on cooperation and trust.
As for me, even as a blogger, I resolutely lend my credence to this view of “power” as so presented above by Dacher Keltner. What about you? Feel free to comment freely!
No comments:
Post a Comment